Vaccines Cause Autism – Part 2: Interleukin-6 and Autism

September 16 2016

If you’ve been following our blog, you know that we recently initiated the hosting of a series of blogs, with permission of the author, from the website Vaccine Papers that compile the scientific proof that vaccines can and do cause autism. If you recall, Part 1 was about how immune activation (whether in pregnant mothers or in infants during the postnatal period when the brain and neurological system are in a phase of rapid development) can cause autistic behaviors and specific brain changes that have been shown to be characteristic of autism.

Part 2 (the original is located here) continues with the research indicating exactly which component of the immune activation is responsible for autistic behaviors and brain changes. As you will see, experimental research with mice has made it explicitly clear that the presence of a particular cytokine, interleukin-6, in sufficient concentration in the developing brain is a necessary and sufficient condition for the development of autism. That means that with these high concentrations of IL-6, the brain will develop autism; without them, it won’t.

We’re picking up the pace a little by publishing Part 3 at the same time as it covers the scientific experiments that extend the immune activation research to primates, a necessary step as, despite the fact that the mouse research is extremely compelling, mice are not as complex as humans. While there is no evidence that IL-6 behaves differently in mice and humans, it is important to establish that the same immune activation that causes autism in mice can cause autism in primates.

As most of the animal experiments specifically studied prenatal maternal immune activation, the next step in the logical progression towards proof that vaccines administered postnatally to children can cause autism is to show that postnatal immune activation can cause the same sorts of IL-6 concentrations and autistic behaviors  as the maternal immune activation. You will find some of that evidence here and much more in Part 4.

And the final step, of course, is to demonstrate that either vaccination or something contained in vaccines, administered either prenatally to pregnant mothers or postnatally to infants and toddlers, can cause the very same high levels of IL-6 in human brains. In upcoming installments, you will find that there is ample scientific evidence that this does indeed occur.

As before, if there is anything you don’t understand or have a question about, don’t be afraid to ask. We think that it’s important for as many vaccine choice advocates to understand the scientific evidence as possible.


Cytokines are signaling chemicals used by the immune system. There are about 47 known cytokines, organized in six families (interleukins, interferons, tumor necrosis factors, chemokines, etc). When an infection or tissue injury occurs, immune cells release cytokines as a way of coordinating their behavior. Some cytokines tell immune cells where to go; some cytokines tell them what to do. Some cytokines tend to increase immune activity, and some tend to decrease it. The actions and interactions of cytokines are extremely complex and not understood.

Cytokines are also involved in communication between the immune system and the nervous system. The immune and nervous systems are deeply connected, and sometimes described with the single term “neuroimmune” system. This is why vaccine adverse effects (e.g. seizure disorders, GB [Guillain-Barré syndrome], encephalopathy) are often neurological. The interactions between immune and nervous systems are constant and profound. There are scientific journals dedicated to the study of immune-nervous system interactions, and the cytokines that are involved.

Cytokines also play critical roles in brain development. While the brain is growing, neurons are dividing, migrating, forming synapses, trimming and adjusting the synapses, and attaching together. Cytokines influence all of these processes.

Proper brain development is critically dependent on the right balance of cytokines. Cytokine levels are tightly and accurately regulated in the brain, especially during brain development. If cytokines are too high or low, or not present in the right ratios, the brain will be defective.

It has been known for years that disturbances in cytokine levels during brain development cause lifelong defects in brain function and brain damage. This area of research has been expanding dramatically in recent years. Here are a couple review papers in the field:

Paper: “Cytokines and Central Nervous System Development (by Patterson)
Paper: “Maternal Immune Activation and Abnormal Brain Development Across CNS Disorders

Interleukin-6 (IL-6)
Interleukin-6 (IL-6) is a well-known cytokine that has important roles in immune function and brain development. IL-6 stimulates fever during sickness. Animals lacking IL-6, or dosed with IL-6 antibodies, are easily killed by minor infections. High IL-6 is implicated as a cause of human autism.

The Vargas study [referred to in Part 1] showed that IL-6 is increased in autistic brain. This is not evidence of causation, of course. But it does mean that IL-6 is a viable candidate for causing autism.

Screen Shot 2015-03-04 at 3.52.46 PM
Above: IL-6 is elevated in numerous areas of the autistic brain. This is not evidence of causation, of course. From Vargas et al. 

Another study by Wei showed that IL-6 was elevated in the autistic cerebellum (location of Purkinje cells) specifically.

IL6 in cerebellum
Above: IL-6 is elevated in autistic brain, and the cerebellum. This is where the Purkinje cells are located. This is not evidence of causation, of course. From Wei et al. 

Also, this study (Full paper: “The Combined Role of Serotonin and Interleukin-6 as Biomarker for Autism), shows that IL-6 blood levels correlate with autism severity scores, and proposes the use of IL-6 (in combination with serotonin), as a blood test for autism. This is also not evidence of causation, but of course it does establish causation plausibility.

Of course, mere correlation cannot prove causation, but correlation does imply possibility. For evidence of causation, we turn to controlled animal experiments.

Patterson’s Remarkable Experiment
Dr. Patterson pioneered the poly(I:C) experiments which showed that autism-like behavior and brain damage are caused by immune activation. It is known that poly(I:C) immune activation stimulates IL-6 production.

In order to test if IL-6 was the cause of the behavioral changes and brain damage, Dr. Patterson performed a series of experiments comparing the following treatments on pregnant mice, and then testing the offspring:
1) poly(I:C) immune activation
2) poly(I:C) immune activation + IL-6 antibody
3) IL-6 injection
4) saline controls
5) controls with other cytokine injections (IL-1a, TNFa, IFNg for all you nerds out there)
6) poly(I:C) in mice lacking the gene for IL-6 (“IL-6 knockout” mice)

Poly(I:C) immune activation causes an increase in IL-6. But it also causes unknown other things and cytokine effects. In order to determine the effect of IL-6 in isolation, the IL-6 is injected alone in one group. In order to determine if there are other aspects of immune activation causing the damage, the poly(I:C) is combined with an antibody for IL-6. The IL-6 antibody selectively neutralizes the IL-6 and leaves all other consequences of immune activation unaffected. The IL-6 knockout mice provide confirmation by a different method.

The “other cytokine” controls were used to determine if perhaps other cytokines induced by immune activation might also have an effect. The other cytokines were tested alone and by combining poly(I:C) with their neutralizing antibodies.

Of course, all these groups were compared with saline-injected controls.

The results were amazingly consistent. IL-6 alone was shown to be both necessary and sufficient for autism-like brain damage. All of it. IL-6 knockout mice and IL-6-antibody mice were normal, even though they were exposed to poly(I:C) immune activation. The autism behaviors and damage could not be reproduced without IL-6, and IL-6 alone caused the same amount of damage as immune activation. Patterson wrote:

IL-6 is central to the process by which maternal immune activation causes long-term behavioral alterations in the offspring.

and

. . . blocking IL-6 prevents >90% of the changes seen in offspring of poly(I:C)-injected females, showing that gene expression changes, as well as behavioral changes, are normalized by eliminating IL-6 from the maternal immune response.

and (in a separate paper)

IL-6 is necessary and sufficient to mediate these effects since the effects . . . are prevented by injection of pregnant mice with poly(I:C) combined with an anti-IL-6 antibody, and are mimicked by a single maternal injection of IL-6.

The results were crystal clear: IL-6 alone is responsible for causing the autism-like alterations in behavior. Such specificity is fairly unusual in these types of studies.

Paper (Smith et al.): “Maternal Immune Activation Alters Fetal Brain Development through Interleukin-6

Here are some of the detailed results:
MIA IL-6 causation proof copy
Above: Results of social preference test. IL-6 antibody prevented social preference change in offspring. IFNg antibody (and others) did not prevent social preference change. Autism brain damage is caused by IL-6, and only IL-6. From Smith et al. 

Replicated and Extended
Patterson’s results with IL-6 have been replicated and expanded upon by other researchers. For example, an international team of researchers (Pineda et al.) replicated Patterson’s results with IL-6. They reported that IL-6 caused autism behaviors of low sociability and impaired communication and that IL-6 antibody provided protection. They also reported that IL-6 was both “necessary and sufficient” for autism-like brain damage.

Paper (Pineda et al): “Maternal Immune Activation Promotes Hippocampal Kindling Epileptogenesis in Mice

This research group also discovered an independent effect of the cytokine “interleukin-1-beta” (IL-1B). IL-6 and IL-1B caused epilepsy (and autism behavior) when combined. IL-6 by itself caused autism behavior, without epilepsy.

This is an important finding because about 10-30% of human autistics also have epilepsy.

The researchers provided a handy chart summarizing their results:
IL-6, IL-1B AUTISM EPILEPSY
Above: IL-6 is necessary and sufficient for autism. IL-6+IL-1B is necessary and sufficient for epilepsy, and causes autism behavior as well. From Pineda et al. 

Quick Word About Timing (prenatal vs. postnatal exposure)
All the above studies were done using maternal immune activation. The offspring are exposed to IL-6 as fetuses (in the womb), by stimulating immune activation in the mother. The timing issue (i.e. prenatal vs. postnatal exposure) is considered thoroughly in Part 4.

Postnatal Exposure to IL-6 by Engineered Virus
A different research group (Dr. Wei et al., at the New York Institute for Basic Research) introduced IL-6 to the brain in a different way: by infection with genetically engineered virus. The virus (carrying an IL-6 gene) was injected into the mouse brain after birth. When the virus infects brain cells, the brain cells produce IL-6. There was no prenatal/fetal exposure to immune activation or IL-6. All the exposure to IL-6 occurred after birth.

Control mice were injected with the same virus lacking the IL-6 gene. This is a proper control that isolates the effect of IL-6.

Paper (Wei et al): “Brain IL-6 Elevation Causes Neuronal Circuitry Imbalances and Mediates Autism-Like Behaviors

The postnatal exposure to IL-6 caused autism-like behaviors and brain damage. Dr. Wei also observed other important changes in neurological activity. This is a very important study, so the results are considered in detail.

Postnatal IL-6 exposure by engineered virus caused changes in social behavior. IL-6-exposed mice displayed less interest in social interaction.
Wei social IL6

Also, IL-6 exposure (postnatal) by engineered virus caused significantly slower water maze navigation, indicating impairment in memory and learning. From Wei et al. 

Wei maze test
Above: IL-6 exposure caused poor learning and memory function in the water maze test. From Wei et al. 

Wei stated

We demonstrated that IL-6 is an important mediator of autism-like behaviors. Mice with an elevated IL-6 in brain developed autism-like behaviors, including impaired cognition ability, deficits in learning, abnormal anxiety-like trait and habituation, as well as a decreased social interaction initiated at later stages. These findings suggest that an IL-6 elevation in the brain could modulate certain pathological alterations and contribute to the development of autism.

Note that the engineered virus method of IL-6 exposure tested the effect of IL-6 in isolation. The numerous consequences of poly(I:C) immune activation are not present, and the control group received the same virus. Also, the IL-6 exposure occurred exclusively after birth. In this experiment,there was no prenatal/fetal exposure or maternal involvement whatsoever.

Prepulse Inhibition and Excessive Excitation in Wei et al.
An important characteristic of autism (and other mental illness like schizophrenia) is a reduction in what is known as “prepulse inhibition.” Prepulse inhibition is the phenomenon of a first sudden sound causing a reduction in the startle response of a second sound a fraction of a second later. So, for example, if someone behind you makes a clicking sound, you will be less startled by a loud hand clap occurring a moment later. This phenomenon is the result of constant balancing between excitation and inhibition in brain circuits. The autistic brain is not properly balanced between excitation and inhibition. In autism, the brain is biased towards excessive excitation by having an excess of excitatory synapses. The result is that autistics are constantly overstimulated by sounds and other sensory input. Autistics have unusually low or absent “prepulse inhibition.” Autistics are constantly startled by things happening around them; a prepulse has no (or small) effect on reducing the startle response.

Dr. Patterson did extensive studies of prepulse inhibition in immune activation. Patterson reported that poly(I:C) and IL-6 both caused an absence of prepulse inhibition. Prepulse inhibition absence is an important characteristic of autism (and schizophrenia).

Dr. Wei reported that IL-6 from the engineered virus (creating IL-6 exposure starting after birth) caused a reduction in prepulse inhibition.

Wei IL6 prepulse inhibition
Above: Postnatal IL-6 exposure caused disappearance of prepulse inhibition. Above data is from electrical testing of neurons in brain slices, not live mice. From Wei et al. ***=P<0.001 

Wei also investigated the reason for the absence of prepulse inhibition. This was done by measuring the numbers of excitatory and inhibitory synapses. It was discovered that IL-6 exposure caused a highly significant increase in excitatory synapses, and a highly significant decrease in inhibitory synapses.
Wei IL6 imbalances
Above: IL-6 exposure caused increase in excitatory synapses and decrease in inhibitory synapses. The hippocampus and somatosensory cortex are brain regions involved in processing sensory input. So an excess of excitatory synapses and deficit of inhibitory synapses explains why autistics are so sensitive to light and sound. From Wei et al. ***=P<0.001, **=P<0.01

This imbalance of excitatory and inhibitory synapses explains the lack of prepulse inhibition. It also explains the well-known sensitivity of autistics to sounds, lights, movement and other sensory stimulation. There is evidence that autistics have excitatory/inhibitory imbalances, though direct measurements of synapse types have not yet been performed on human brains.

Wei stated

A growing number of studies suggest that autism is likely to arise from functional changes in neural circuitry and to be associated with an imbalance between excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission . . .

Excitation/Inhibition Imbalance in Human Autistics
The generally accepted belief that human autistics have an excitatory/inhibitory imbalance is based on circumstantial evidence. It’s consistent with what is known about autistic behavior and animal models, but there has not been direct experimental evidence for it until now (December 2015). Robertson et al. report experimental evidence for excitatory/inhibitory imbalance in autistic subjects. In this experiment, two different images are presented to a subject, one to each eye. In this situation, the left (L) and right (R) visual circuits compete for perceptual dominance. The subject’s attention switches back and forth between the images. This “perceptual switching” requires inhibition of the active visual circuit (the L or R circuit). Since autistics are less able to suppress neuron activity, they report much less frequent perceptual switching. Normal control subjects are able to switch their perception much more rapidly and easily. Also, autistics are less able to fully inhibit perception of an image. Robertson et al. state

An imbalance between excitatory/inhibitory neurotransmission has been posited as a central characteristic of the neurobiology of autism . . . .

and

We demonstrate a robust, replicated autistic deficit in binocular rivalry, a basic visual function that is thought to rely on the balance of excitation/inhibition in visual cortex . . . .

and

These results suggest a disruption in inhibitory signaling in the autistic brain . . . .

Paper (Robertson): “Reduced GABAergic Action in the Autistic Brain

IL-6 in Mice and Humans
Some vaccine advocates have objected that IL-6 may have different roles in humans and mouse models. This objection is contradicted by scientific evidence and our understanding of the role of cytokines in brain development. For example, Brown et al. state the following:

It is also noteworthy that IL-6 response is quite conserved across species [71-73] and that the general central nervous system development in rodents is governed by the same basic principles as in humans [74].

This view is widely accepted. It is not reasonable to argue that the evidence showing that IL-6 causes autism is not relevant to humans.

Paper (Brown et al.): “Metabolic Consequences of Interleukin-6 Challenge in Developing Neurons and Astroglia

Also, there are no known differences in the functions of IL-6 in mice and humans. This paper reviews differences between human and mouse immunology. IL-6 is not mentioned.
Paper (Mestas et al.): “Differences between Mouse and Human Immunology

Conclusion
The evidence shows that IL-6 almost certainly causes autism. IL-6 explains all the autism behaviors, damage to Purkinje cells, mitochondrial dysfunction, the absence of prepulse inhibition, and the excess of excitatory synapses. Other known causes of autism likely act through IL-6 stimulation.

Further, postnatal IL-6 exposure causes autistic behavior. The immune activation need not occur in the womb. The issue of postnatal vs. prenatal immune activation is discussed thoroughly in Part 4.

There are now review papers on IL-6 and autism. For further reading:
Brain IL-6 and Autism
Maternal Immune Activation and Autism Spectrum Disorder: Interleukin-6 Signaling as a Key Mechanistic Pathway

Part 3 in this series describes immune activation experiments with monkeys:

Click here for a list of papers in this post.

Click here to go on to Part 3: Immune Activation Causes Autism in Monkeys.

Click here to see a listing of the other blogs in this series.


Pin It
This entry was posted in Blogs by Thinking Moms' Revolution, Vaccines Cause Autism and tagged , , , . Bookmark the permalink.

3 Responses to Vaccines Cause Autism – Part 2: Interleukin-6 and Autism

  1. Mandy says:

    Please link to Part 1 on this page.

  2. Hans Scholl says:

    But I thought the vaccine to autism link had been “debunked” by Poul Thoresen in Denmark , and by Colleen Boyle & Frank DeStefano in 2003 of the CDC\EIS .
    And the BBC told me so too and they never lie .

    “debunked” – what a word . crikey

    This is all so confusing

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *